Does testosterone increase mental mistakes?
Colin Camerer, professor of behavioral economics at the California University of Technology, examines this question.
Colin Camerer is a pioneer in behavioral economics and in neuroeconomics. He is interested in how psychological forces and their deeper neuroscientific foundations influence economic decisions involving individuals and markets. In his research, he uses experiments to better understand how individuals and markets function, neuroscience to gain insight into the neuroscientific drivers for decision making and behavior, and game theory. For example, he’s exploring why price bubbles form, when they crash, and how people value immediate and future rewards and costsâespecially those that create temptation. He wants to understand strategic situations such as when other people’s choices affect an individual and how someone anticipates what others will do.
Camerer was on the faculty of the Graduate School of Business at the University of Chicago from 1991 to 1994, the Wharton School of the University of Pennsylvania from 1983 to 1991, and the Kellogg Graduate School of Business at Northwestern University from 1981 to 1983. He was a visiting fellow at Stanford University from 1997 to 1998, a visiting scholar at the Russell Sage Foundation between 1991 and 1992, and a visiting professor of business at Caltech in 1987.
He is a member of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences (2003), a fellow of the Econometric Society (1993), and a fellow of the Society for the Advancement of Economic Theory (2011). He was president of the Society for Neuroeconomics (2005â2006) and president of the Economic Science Association (2001â2003). Since 2007, he has been a chair of the Russell Sage Foundation Behavioral Economics Roundtable. He has one patent accepted on “Active Learning Decision Engines.” He is on editorial boards for numerous journals. In 2013, he was named a MacArthur Fellow.
Testosterone
Testosterone is a hormone that affects reproduction and all the drama surrounding it. When a male animal comes across an attractive mate– or a threatening competitor– testosterone is released into the bloodstream and brain.
The ideal way to test its effects is to change testosterone in one group of men and see how they behave differently compared to a control group. Thatâs exactly what we did in our research. We recruited 243 men. We either increased their testosterone, by having them apply a gel to their skin, or had them apply a âplaceboâ gel that feels the same but has no active ingredients. (They couldnât tell which gel they had gotten.)
Then the men all took a test. The test used three brain teasers to measure âcognitive reflectionâ. Hereâs one of the questions: Suppose there is a small amount of algae in a pond. The algae doubles every day, and completely covers the pond after 48 days. On what day is the pond half-covered in algae? The intuitive response is that the pond is half-covered in 24 days, because 24 is half of 48. But thatâs wrong. The correct answer is that the pond is half-covered on the 47th day: because the algae doubles every day, it must be half-covered on day 47, in order to be fully covered on day 48.
The men who had their testosterone increased made more mistakes on these brain teasers. They got only about 1-1/2 right, and they answered fast when they were wrong.
The placebo subjects got 2 right.
Testosterone seems to increase mental confidence, which is bad when you are solving brain teasers. But the biological purpose of testosterone is maintaining male status for reproductionâwhy would it lead to more mental mistakes? Our hunch is that testosterone increases mental confidence, which increases status, which must lead to reproductive opportunities somehow. So testosterone is doing its number 1 job after allâenhancing reproductionâthrough a roundabout route.
Comments
One response to “Colin Camerer, California Institute of Technology – Testosterone”
Has this test been replicated independently from another research group. What number of participants were used and how many independent group tests were done. This result seems possible, but I think it must be independently verified before moving it to a real scientific conclusion. These results of a half question off seems almost a statistical error. Why not have 10 questions or more? My guess it would not hold up with more testing. Ever since the NPR article, I have become more skeptical of this type of simple studies that could be just noise of false positives. I would like to see a full study done in a more controlled situation and with less simple metrics. See NPR at: http://www.npr.org/2015/08/28/435416046/research-results-often-fail-to-be-replicated-researchers-say
I personally have a pituitary issue which makes me interested in this facts of this situation.
[email protected]